An image is created on a slide as the result of a chemical process. The light arriving on the film through the lenses causes some chemical reactions on the emulsion that is on the film, and an image is formed. The film is then processed, and eventually processed once more to obtain photographic prints.
Various films are often described using an ``H-D'' plot (H-D stands for Hurter and Driffield who devised them in 1890 [JaMe66]). An H-D plot describes a density as a response to a given exposure. Let us define exposure and density next.
Exposure is defined as the product of the irradiance incident upon
the photosensitive surface (I) and the time during which the
surface is exposed (t):
Photographic exposure is usually given in photometric rather than
radiometric units. In equation 3.1, I is given in
or lux and exposure is then in lux-sec. In the cases where the
incoming flux is a continuous spectral distribution, the exposure is
given by the integral:
The measured response of a photographic material is given in density. Density is a unitless, logarithmic measure that indicates the opacity of an emulsion that results from processing.
Transmission density is used for describing the response of
photographic film. It is defined as:
where T is a transmittance value, , that gives the ratio
of light transmitted through an emulsion to the quantity of light
incident to it. There is also the reflective density
which is
used in describing the response of photographic papers.
Let us consider now, a color slide with characteristic curves as in fig. 3.3. The horizontal axis shows the logarithm of the exposure, the vertical axis shows the densities in the r,g,b channels.
The density ranges of the three color channels are somewhat
different from another, hence an achromatic gray can only be produced
in the coincident part of the density ranges. It can be seen in
fig. 3.3 that the density ranges exceed 3. In linear terms:
the contrast exceeds 1000! The corresponding illumination range on the
horizontal axis is [-2.5, -0.1]. Values less then -2.5 and greater
then -0.1 cause no change in densities (so called ``fog'' and
``flare'' regions). That means, roughly, that the logarithm of the
range of exposition is 2.4, a value of about 250 on a linear
scale! This is much higher than the displayable effective contrast on
any other medium. Dynamic ranges of black and white glossy photo
papers can be up to 60, but for all other media including color
photos and prints it is significantly smaller, in a range of 10 to
40. In photography, only the straight curve sections are
convenient. Moreover, the standard is a paper print made from the
slide, perceptionally correct for a contrast rendering of 1:1. This
means that we want to reproduce the original contrast of 50 as 50 on the
final print. Therefore, the really applicable range of contrast for
exposures does not exceed the value of 45, that is about 1.65 in
(actually the recommended contrast value in photography is
32). For instance, on the horizontal axis of fig. 3.3 the
section [-0.25,-1.9] may be applied. This corresponds to the density
range [0.2, 2.7] on the vertical axis. That means that the original
scene contrast of
is mapped to a contrast of
on
the slide.
Figure 3.3: The characteristic curves of Kodak Ektachrome Panther
100x Professional
Slide gamma is defined as the slope of the straight part of the
characteristic seen in fig. 3.3. Slide gamma values vary from
1.5 to 1.7, and this is the reason why the scene contrast is enlarged
on the slide. The question emerges why the gamma value is not
1. Obviously, on a paper print (enlargement) gamma must be 1,
otherwise the print would not be equivalent to the original
appearance. Remember that we want to reproduce original contrast
exactly. This is possible since photo papers have gamma values as
well. Enlargements from slides are made on so called positive papers,
with gamma values less than 1. The resultant gamma is the product of
the gammas of the slide and of the positive paper, in fact, about
1. Actually the original scene contrast will be enlarged on slide, and
then again decreased by the use of a positive photo paper. Similarly,
soft color negatives, with gamma values less than 1, and enlargement
papers of hard gradation (with gamma values over 1) have resultant
gamma values of about 1. The original contrast is changed on slides
due to a value of 1.5 to 1.7. Even the color components are
changed from r, g, b to
,
,
. The question emerges how it is possible that slides are, despite
all these facts, perceived correctly when projected. A projected slide
enlarges the original contrast, but the perceived image looks correct.
The Goldberg rule gives us the answer [Schr81]. The Goldberg rule
states that the way we see something depends in an exponential way
(Goldberg-Gamma) on the surrounding lighting. If the human eye is
adapted to a very dark surrounding, a Goldberg-Gamma value of 1.5 to
1.7 gives perceptually correct results. For eyes adapted to brightness
the Goldberg-Gamma decreases down to 1. That is one of the reasons why
slides viewed in a well lit room do not look satisfactory, but when
they are projected in a dark room they are just perfect. In other
words, our visual system reduces the contrast in the dark surrounding,
and high contrast projection is perceived as normal contrast image. On
the other hand if a slide is viewed in a bright surrounding, its
contrast is too high, and it looks somehow contrast-less. This paradox
that high contrast image seems to have low contrast, and a low
contrast image looks as full of contrast will be described in the
section 4.2.
Note, that it would be possible to exploit slide characteristic in another way. We could compute a raw image, and compress the original contrast of let's say 300 to 50, and use it as input to the film writer. The resulting slide will then increase input contrast to the original 300 value. In this case the slide should be viewed in a well lit room, placed on a diffuse light source. If there were a big enough slide, it would outperform all common display media this way.